New non-stick toilet bowl

Scientists at Huazhong University of Science and Technology in Wuhan, China, have created a new type of ultra-slippery toilet bowl, making it impossible for anything to stick to it. The new toilet bowl design could eventually replace porcelain and ceramic toilet bowls if they scaled it up.

The ultra-slippering toilet bowl was designed to be a 3D-printed item almost nothing can stick to the bowl. Even more intriguing, the researchers found that the toilet bowl remained slippery, even after being used multiple times and rubbed down with sandpaper.

One of the biggest advantages to having an ultra-slipper toilet bowl isn’t just its cleanliness. If nothing sticks to the bowl, then that means we could also see reduced water usage for flushing the toilet when using this type of material.

The researchers call it an abrasion-resistant super-slippery flush toilet (ARSFT), and it is made using materials that easily repel complex fluids and viscoelastic solids. However, these types of materials are usually easily broken by mechanical abrasions. The material used to make the ultra-slippery toilet bowl, though, can even stand up to sandpaper and remain just as slippery as it did before.

Writeup Paper

 
They're really gonna need to work on that acronym... :lol:
 
Cold Medicine Decongestant Found Ineffective
BY JARED S. HOPKINS

Your favorite cold medicine for a stuffy nose may soon be unavailable.
An advisory panel to the Food and Drug Administration declared Tuesday that an ingredient in widely used oral decongestants doesn’t work, setting the stage for dozens of products to be removed from U.S. store shelves. At issue is phenylephrine, an almost century-old ingredient in versions of Benadryl, Mucinex, Tylenol and other over-the-counter pills, syrups and liquids to clear up congested noses. Phenylephrine, first permitted for use in 1938, didn’t go through the rigorous clinical trials that regulators require today for medications, and more recent studies found the ingredient to be ineffective at relieving congestion. The latest research prompted pharmacists and physicians to call for ending sales of the drugs. “I do not believe that the evidence that was presented supports in any way the efficacy of this product remaining on the market,” said Diane Ginsburg, a panel member and associate dean for Healthcare Partnerships at the University of Texas at Austin College of Pharmacy. “We really should not have products on the market that are not effective.”

The FDA panel’s unanimous vote clears the way for the agency to remove oral phenylephrine from its list of approved over-the-counter ingredients. That would mean products containing the ingredient couldn’t be sold in the U.S. The FDA doesn’t have to follow the recommendations of its advisory panels, but it often does. Over-the-counter products that treat cough, sinus and flu symptoms, including phenylephrine pills, generated about $5 billion in sales in 2021, according to research firm IRI.
The Wall Street Journal reported last year that some recent studies found oral phenylephrine in certain medicines was ineffective at relieving nasal congestion from a cold, flu or allergies. The FDA said in an analysis before the panel’s meeting that the oral phenylephrine formulations are safe but ineffective at standard or even higher doses.

The agency said that three large recent industry-funded studies evaluating medicines with phenylephrine by manufacturers found that people who took medicines with phenylephrine fared no better than those who received a placebo. The agency also found that research from decades ago didn’t meet current clinical trial design standards and included inconsistent results. The Consumer Healthcare Products Association, an industry group, said at the hearing that people rely on the medicines and that they should remain on the market, stating that the older research shows it is effective. “The bottom line is that oral phenylephrine is safe and that it works,” said Marcia Howard, CHPA’s vice president of regulatory and science affairs. If the FDA found phenylephrine ineffective, manufacturers could potentially reformulate their products or submit applications as new drugs, depending on the data supporting whether phenylephrine works in the product, a CHPA spokesman said.
Physicians and pharmacists say that because oral phenylephrine is metabolized in the gut and liver, it can’t reach the bloodstream in sufficient levels and cause the blood vessels to narrow and provide relief. “The fact that some patients think they are getting relief from specifically oral phenylephrine can be a placebo effect,” said panel member Dr. Mark Dykewicz, an allergy and immunology professor at Saint Louis University School of Medicine. Some panel members also said no further study of phenylephrine is needed.

Kenvue , which sells Tylenol and Benadryl, didn’t respond to requests for comment. The company’s Sudafed PE also contains phenylephrine. Reckitt Benckiser Group, which makes Mucinex, didn’t respond to requests for comment.
Instead of taking pills that contain phenylephrine to clear congestion, people can take pills containing pseudoephedrine, antihistamines, or nasal spray products, including those with phenylephrine, which physicians say are effective.
Phenylephrine is now in more than 260 oral nose and sinus medicines, according to a 2020 paper published in JAMA Otolaryngology–Head & Neck Surgery.

Where the Ingredient Is Used
Some common over-the-counter medicines with phenylephrine include:
  • Advil Sinus Congestion & Pain
  • Benadryl Allergy Plus Congestion for Sinus Pres-sure & Nasal Congestion Relief
  • DayQuil Cold & Flu
  • Flonase Headache & Allergy Relief
  • Mucinex Maximum Strength Sinus-Max Pres-sure, Pain & Cough Liquid Gels
  • NyQuil Cold & Flu
  • Robitussin Night-time Severe Multi- Symptom Cough, Cold + Flu Syrup
  • Sudafed PE Sinus Congestion
  • Theraflu Daytime Severe Cold Relief Berry Burst Flavor Hot Liquid Powder
  • Tylenol Sinus + Head-ache Non-Drowsy Daytime Caplets for Nasal Congestion, Sinus Pressure & Pain Relief
University of Florida pharmacy researchers who reviewed testing of the pills asked the FDA, in a citizen’s petition filed in 2015, to remove phenylephrine from the list of approved over-the-counter medicines. The drug was used in over-the counter products starting at least in the 1950s. In 1976, the FDA included phenylephrine, along with two other main decongestant ingredients called phenylpropanolamine and pseudoephedrine, as over-the counter products when it overhauled its regulations.

In 2000, the FDA asked manufacturers to remove phenylpropanolamine over concerns about an association with hemorrhagic stroke. Six years later, Congress restricted sales of products containing pseudoephedrine to behind the pharmacy counter because the ingredient can be used to make methamphetamine.

WSJ
 
SSTB: super slippery toilet bowl
SDSTB: hmmm.....
 
Human trials of artificial wombs could start soon

A hairless, pale-skinned lamb lies on its side in what appears to be an oversized sandwich bag filled with hazy fluid. Its eyes are closed, and its snout and limbs jerk as if the animal — which is only about three-quarters of the way through its gestation period — is dreaming.

The lamb was one of eight in a 2017 artificial-womb experiment carried out by researchers at the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (CHOP) in Pennsylvania. When the team published its research1 in April of that year, it released a video of the experiments that spread widely and captured imaginations — for some, evoking science-fiction fantasies of humans being conceived and grown entirely in a laboratory.

Now, the researchers at CHOP are seeking approval for the first human clinical trials of the device they’ve been testing, named the Extra-uterine Environment for Newborn Development, or EXTEND. The team has emphasized that the technology is not intended — or able — to support development from conception to birth. Instead, the scientists hope that simulating some elements of a natural womb will increase survival and improve outcomes for extremely premature babies. In humans, that’s anything earlier than 28 weeks of gestation — less than 70% of the way to full term, which is typically between 37 and 40 weeks.

The CHOP group has made bold predictions about the technology’s potential. In another 2017 video describing the project, Alan Flake, a fetal surgeon at CHOP who has been leading the effort, said: “If it’s as successful as we think it can be, ultimately, the majority of pregnancies that are predicted at-risk for extreme prematurity would be delivered early onto our system rather than being delivered premature onto a ventilator.” In 2019, several members of the CHOP team joined a start-up company, Vitara Biomedical in Philadelphia, which has since raised US$100 million to develop EXTEND. (Flake declined to comment for this article, citing “conflicts of interest” and “restrictions on proprietary information”. His co-authors on the 2017 paper did not respond to Nature’s request for comment.)

The US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) will convene a meeting of independent advisers on 19–20 September to discuss regulatory and ethical considerations and what human trials for the technology might look like. The committee’s discussion will be scrutinized by the handful of other groups around the world that are developing similar devices, and by bioethicists exploring the implications for health equity, reproductive rights and more.

“This is definitely an exciting step and it’s been a long time coming,” says Kelly Werner, a bioethicist and neonatologist at Columbia University Medical Center in New York City, who is not affiliated with groups developing artificial-womb technology. “Clinicians who work with premature babies will be closely following this meeting,” she says.

Early start

Preterm birth, defined by the World Health Organization as birth before 37 weeks of gestation, can happen spontaneously or because some conditions — such as an infection, hormone imbalance, high blood pressure or diabetes — can turn the womb into an inhospitable environment for the fetus.

It poses an enormous global health problem. Preterm birth is the largest cause of death and disability in children under five. In 2020, there were about 13.4 million such births worldwide, and complications related to preterm birth caused about 900,000 deaths in 2019.

Mortality is strongly linked with the baby’s gestational age at birth. At or before 22 weeks — considered the cusp of fetal viability — few fetuses survive outside the womb. By 28 weeks, most can survive, but often require significant life support. Artificial-womb technology aims to improve outcomes for preterm babies who are born in the period between 22 and 28 weeks, for whom survival has improved, but long-term health issues are frequent.

In a study2 of 2.5 million people in Sweden, for example, 78% of people born before 28 weeks of gestation had some sort of medical condition — ranging from asthma and hypertension to cerebral palsy and epilepsy — by the time they were adults. For full-term births, that rate was 37%.

Death and disability, especially in babies born at younger gestational ages, often occur because the lungs and brain are among the last organs to fully mature in humans. That’s why obstetricians try to prevent preterm birth whenever possible — the longer fetuses can safely stay in the womb, the higher their odds are of long-term survival and good health.

In a natural womb, a fetus receives oxygen, nutrients, antibodies and hormonal signals and gets rid of waste through the placenta, a transient organ in which fetal blood interacts with maternal blood. Of these various roles, artificial-womb technology is most focused on providing oxygen and removing carbon dioxide, replacing the mechanical ventilators that are often used for neonates. These can damage fragile developing lungs that would otherwise still be filled with amniotic fluid.

The artificial womb “would bridge a baby born extremely premature through those days and weeks when they’re most at risk for lung and brain damage”, Werner says. The CHOP group has signalled that it would wean babies off its system after a few weeks, when their organs are more fully developed and their likelihood of healthy survival is higher.

The group’s system would work by placing extremely premature babies into what it calls a Biobag, filled with an electrolyte-laden fluid designed to mimic amniotic fluid. Surgeons would connect the blood vessels in the umbilical cord to a system that oxygenates blood outside the body. The fetal heart would still pump blood as it does in the natural womb.

Making the connection with blood vessels in the umbilical cord is difficult, because the arteries are tiny and begin to contract as a baby is delivered. So, surgeons will need to hook up the vessels to the system within minutes. The process “has got to be really slick”, requiring deft surgical skills and speedy transitions, says Anna David, a maternal–fetal specialist at University College London.

Flake and his colleagues have been testing the system on lambs, which are often used in fetal research because they are developmentally similar to humans. Sheep typically gestate for about 5 months; the lambs that the researchers used were the equivalent of a human fetus at 23 weeks of gestation. In 2017, the team reported that it kept eight lambs alive for up to four weeks using the artificial womb1. In that time, the animals sprouted wool and their lungs and brains grew to maturity. After four weeks, the researchers euthanized the animals so they could study how the system had affected organ development.

Since 2017, the researchers have been testing various ways to connect the animals to the oxygenation machine, and they have been in conversation with the FDA about starting clinical trials.

Varying approaches

Researchers who spoke to Nature say that the CHOP group’s system is probably closest to human trials. But groups in Spain, Japan, Australia, Singapore and the Netherlands are also developing artificial-womb technology. A team led by fetal surgeon George Mychaliska at University of Michigan Health in Ann Arbor refers to its device as an artificial placenta. And even though in practice it serves the same purpose as EXTEND, the teams’ approaches are starkly different (see ‘Life support’).

The Michigan device doesn’t surround babies with fluid, but instead fills only their lungs though an endotracheal tube. And it uses a pump to draw blood from the jugular vein, oxygenate it outside the body and send it back in through the umbilical vein; the CHOP group hooks its device up to both the umbilical arteries and the vein.

Each approach comes with its own pros and cons, which the groups highlighted in a pair of commentaries in July3,4. Currently, the CHOP device necessitates delivery by caesarean section, because the umbilical arteries start closing quickly during birth, and natural labour can take a long time. But the risks of an elective c-section for a pregnant person are not trivial and must be factored in to the equation, David says. In its July article3, the CHOP group acknowledges this risk, but notes that up to 55% of extremely premature babies are already born by c-section.

By contrast, clinicians using Michigan’s approach could deliver a premature baby naturally and determine whether the infant can breathe unaided. If not, they could still hook it up to the system, because the umbilical vein doesn’t close as quickly as the arteries do, says Robert Bartlett, a surgeon at the University of Michigan who works with Mychaliska. But the external pump for moving blood around carries a risk of straining the heart or causing brain bleeds. According to published data, the Michigan group has so far sustained lambs for about two weeks5, compared with the CHOP group’s four weeks. (Mychaliska did not respond to e-mails asking for comment.)

Although the groups disagree on the best approach, Bartlett says he hopes the CHOP group successfully secures FDA approval for human trials. He and his colleagues at Michigan plan to seek FDA approval in about a year, he adds.

Eduard Gratacós, a fetal-medicine specialist at the University of Barcelona in Spain who is also developing an artificial womb, acknowledges that his group is years behind the CHOP group. But if clinical-trial results look promising, he says, “we’re going to need several of these systems in the world”.

A leap from lambs

Even as excitement mounts about this type of technology, however, questions remain about what data will be required to get the green light for human trials. “It’s a big leap to go to humans from lambs,” Gratacós says.

Lambs at the same stage of development as extremely premature babies are two to three times larger, meaning that researchers would need to further tweak the already-tiny equipment necessary for the artificial womb. Fetal pigs are more similar in size to human fetuses, but they are harder to work with than lambs, Bartlett says. Non-human primates are a gold-standard animal model to precede clinical trials because of their physiological similarities to humans, but their fetuses are even smaller than those of humans, and the ethics of conducting such experiments are complex.

Guid Oei, an obstetrician at Eindhoven University of Technology in the Netherlands, and his colleagues have been developing their own artificial-womb system alongside simulation dolls that allow clinicians to practice transferring a fetus. “You only have one chance to do it right, and the learning curve should not be on actual human beings,” he says.

Nevertheless, to Matthew Kemp, an obstetrician at the National University of Singapore, “the data aren’t there from an ethical position” to justify the launch of human trials, unless “someone is sitting on a bunch of data that isn’t published”. Kemp, who is also developing an artificial-womb system, hopes to see data on how the experimental animals fare in the long term, as well as data from non-human primates, before clinical trials begin. (The CHOP group hinted at “extensive unpublished data prepared for regulatory approval” in its July commentary3.) “It’s a new treatment modality,” Kemp says. “The bottom line is they’ve got to make a really strong case that it’s better and safer in the short and long term” than the current life-saving measures used.

Ethics and more

Safety questions won’t be the only ethical concerns. The development of artificial wombs represents a “big transformational leap” that “solves lots of issues”, says David. But, she adds, “it also opens up a whole new slew of issues”. After the 2017 study1 generated extensive media coverage, fears spread that artificial wombs could one day replace pregnancy.

But researchers discount these concerns. This idea “is so far in the distant future that it’s not worth discussing its implications in relation to the current technology”, Werner says.

Those developing artificial wombs in the United States will also have to contend with a politically charged environment for reproductive rights. Flake and Mychaliska have been careful not to give any indication that an artificial womb could change the definition of fetal viability — which has enormous implications after the US Supreme Court struck down the 1973 landmark abortion decision Roe v. Wade in June last year. Previously, the 1973 ruling had protected abortion until the fetus is viable outside the womb.

Even what to call the entities in these devices is fraught, says Chloe Romanis, a biolawyer at Durham Law School, UK. They’re not fetuses in the conventional sense because they are no longer in the womb, she says. And some argue that they are not neonates, which, by the Latin root of the word, assumes they’ve been born. “The name we give to these new unprecedented patients has implications for rights that the law and society affords,” Werner says. The CHOP group has proposed a new name altogether: fetal neonates, or fetonates for short.

Some researchers also worry that artificial wombs would represent an expensive technological solution to a deeper problem. Michael Harrison, a fetal surgeon at the University of California, San Francisco, sometimes called the ‘father of fetal surgery’, says the data he has seen so far have been promising. But he questions whether it’s worth “throwing all that money and tech” on babies that have a poor likelihood of survival instead of finding ways to improve pregnancy support or standard techniques for preterm critical care, which could reduce the need for artificial-womb technology in the long run.

David agrees, adding that there is insufficient research and funding to understand why women go into labour early and how to prevent it. “We need to get real with this,” she says. “Artificial wombs will impact only a tiny fraction of the problem.”

Bartlett says that systemic measures are important, but he argues that better treatment is urgently needed for extremely preterm babies. “A silver bullet that prevents prematurity doesn’t exist and is unlikely to exist in our lifetimes,” he says. “These technologies are what we need when the systemic measures fail.”


 

Half-million-year-old wooden structure unearthed in Zambia​

The discovery of ancient wooden logs in the banks of a river in Zambia has changed archaeologists' understanding of ancient human life.
Researchers found evidence the wood had been used to build a structure almost half a million years ago.
The findings, published in the journal Nature, suggest stone-age people built what may have been shelters.
"This find has changed how I think about our early ancestors," archaeologist Prof Larry Barham said.

The University of Liverpool scientist leads the Deep Roots of Humanity research project, which excavated and analysed the ancient timber.

The discovery could transform the current belief ancient humans led simple, nomadic lives.

"They made something new, and large, from wood," Prof Barham said.
"They used their intelligence, imagination and skills to create something they'd never seen before, something that had never previously existed."
The researchers also uncovered ancient wooden tools, including digging sticks. But what excited them most were two pieces of wood found at right angles to each other.
"One is lying over the other and both pieces of wood have notches cut into them," University of Aberystwyth archaeologist Prof Geoff Duller said.
"You can clearly see those notches have been cut by stone tools.
"It makes the two logs fit together to become structural objects."

Making fire​

Further analysis confirmed the logs were about 476,000 years old.
Team member Perrice Nkombwe, from the Livingstone Museum, in Zambia, said: "I was amazed to know that woodworking was such a deep-rooted tradition.
"It dawned on me that we had uncovered something extraordinary."
Until now, evidence for the human use of wood has been limited to making fire and crafting tools such as digging sticks and spears.

Luminescence dating​

One of the oldest wooden discoveries was a 400,000-year-old spear in prehistoric sands at Clacton-on-Sea, Essex, in 1911.
Unless it is preserved in very specific conditions, wood simply rots away.

But in the meandering riverbanks above the Kalambo Falls, close to the Zambia-Tanzania border, it was waterlogged and essentially pickled for millennia.
The team measured the age of layers of earth in which it was buried, using luminescence dating.
Grains of rock absorb natural radioactivity from the environment over time - essentially charging up like tiny batteries, as Prof Duller put it.
And that radioactivity can be released and measured by heating up the grains and analysing the light emitted.

The size of the two logs, the smaller of which is about 1.5m (5ft), suggests whoever fitted them together was building something substantial.
Unlikely to have been a hut or permanent dwelling, it could have formed part of a platform for a shelter, the team says.
"It might be some sort of structure to sit beside the river and fish," Prof Duller said:.
"But it's hard to tell what sort of [complete] structure it might have been."

It is also unclear what species of ancient human - or hominid - built it.
No bones have been found at this site so far.
And the timber is much older than the earliest modern human - or Homo sapien - fossils, which are about 315,000 years old.

Woodworking tradition​

"It could have been Homo sapiens and we just haven't discovered fossils from that age yet," Prof Duller said.
"But it could be a different species - Homo erectus or Homo naledi - there were a number of hominid species around at that time in southern Africa."
Transported to the UK for analysis and preservation, the wooden artefacts are being stored in tanks that mimic the waterlogging that preserved them so beautifully for the last half-million years. But they will soon return to Zambia to be displayed.
"With this discovery, we hope to enrich our collection and use the finds to inform the interpretation of the woodworking tradition in Zambia," Ms Nkombwe said.
Continuing the work at the Kalambo Falls site, she added, "has the potential to deepen our knowledge of ancient woodworking techniques, craftsmanship, and human interactions with the environment".
https://www.bbc.com/news/science-environment-66846772
 

Robot suit moves from anime to reality​

For $3.6 million (or 400 million yen), you could become the owner and pilot of a 4.5 metre, 3.2 tonne robot suit, courtesy of Japanese robotics startup Tsubame Industries. CEO Ryo Yoshida says he wanted to build a creation that would combine Japan's renowned strengths in animation, games, robotics and automobiles into a statement that says, 'This is Japan.' Yoshida says he hopes the robot suit can one day be used to help with disaster relief or in the space industry.
https://www.cbc.ca/player/play/2270055491611
 
We count to 4 differently to how we count above 5

For more than a century, researchers have known that people are generally very good at eyeballing quantities of four or fewer items. But performance at sizing up numbers drops markedly — becoming slower and more prone to error — in the face of larger numbers.

Now scientists have discovered why: the human brain uses one mechanism to assess four or fewer items and a different one for when there are five or more. The findings, obtained by recording the neuron activity of 17 human participants, settle a long-standing debate on how the brain estimates how many objects a person sees. The results were published in Nature Human Behaviour on 2 October.

The limits of the human ability to estimate large quantities have puzzled many generations of scientists. In an 1871 Nature article, economist and logician William Stanley Jevons described his investigations into his own counting skills and concluded “that the number five is beyond the limit of perfect discrimination, by some persons at least”.

Some researchers have argued that the brain uses a single estimation system, one that is simply less precise for higher numbers. Others hypothesize that the performance discrepancy arises from there being two separate neuronal systems to quantify objects. But experiments have failed to determine which model is correct.

Then, a team of researchers had a rare opportunity to record the activity of individual neurons inside the brains of people who were awake. All were being treated for seizures at the University Hospital Bonn in Germany, and had microelectrodes inserted in their brains in preparation for surgery.

The authors showed 17 participants images of anywhere from zero to nine dots on a screen for half a second, and asked them whether they had seen an odd or even number of items. As expected, the participants’ answers were much more precise when they saw four or fewer dots.

The researchers had already learned from previous research that there are specialized neurons associated with specific numbers of items. Some fire primarily when presented with one object, others when presented with two objects and so forth.

“The higher the preferred number, the less selective these neurons were,” says co-author Andreas Nieder, an animal physiologist at the University of Tubingen in Germany. For example, neurons specific to three would only fire in response to that number, whereas neurons that prefer eight would respond to eight but also to seven and nine. As a result, people made more mistakes when trying to quantify a larger number of objects.

This suggests two distinct ‘number systems’ in the brain. Nieder was surprised, as he previously thought that there was only one mechanism. “I had a hard time believing that there’s really this dividing line. But, based on these data, I must accept it,” he says.
 
CountGPT
 
New information physics theory is evidence 'we're living in a simulation,' says author

Going by the fact that Elon Musk said there was only one in a billion chance that the world was not simulated, we might save ourselves a lot of time and assume that on this occasion, as on so many others, the 52-year-old rocket bro has made a beeline for the wrong end of the stick.

Nonetheless, in the developing field of information physics, one researcher claims to have found evidence that we are all living in our own rendition of The Matrix, minus the latex, bad acting, and dated cyberpunk references.

Melvin Vopson, associate professor of physics at the UK's Portsmouth University, has developed theory in the field of information physics and applied it to predict genetic mutations in organisms, including viruses, and help judge their potential consequences.

The basis of the theory is analogous to the second law of thermodynamics, which includes the idea that the disorder within an isolated system will increase unless it is acted on by some external force or condition.

In information physics – the idea that studying the information inherent in physical systems produces fruitful results – the trend goes the other way.

Dr Vopson, who spent some of his career at disk drive giant Seagate, has developed the second law of infodynamics, which says that the entropy – or disorder – within information stays the same or decreases over time. The idea has implications across a number of fields, he claims in a paper published by the American Institute of Physics.

In biology, the theory suggests that genetic mutations follow a pattern governed by information entropy, with implications in genetic research, evolutionary biology, genetic therapies, pharmacology, virology, and more. In atomic physics, the paper claims, electrons arrange themselves in a way that minimizes their information entropy, helping science to understand the stability of chemicals. Lastly, in cosmology, the second law of infodynamics can be used to support the idea of an expanding universe.

"The paper also provides an explanation for the prevalence of symmetry in the universe," said Dr Vopson in a prepared statement.

"Symmetry principles play an important role with respect to the laws of nature, but until now there has been little explanation as to why that could be. My findings demonstrate that high symmetry corresponds to the lowest information entropy state, potentially explaining nature's inclination towards it."

But Dr Vopson takes another step, which is where it gets interesting, or far-fetched, depending on your perspective.

He said the fact that physical systems show a tendency towards declining disorder showed "excess information is removed" and "resembles the process of a computer deleting or compressing waste code to save storage space and optimize power consumption. And as a result supports the idea that we're living in a simulation."

In 2003, Nick Bostrom, Oxford University professor and director of the Future of Humanity Institute, published an argument [PDF] suggesting that the future of humanity lies in one of three scenarios: one, that humans will become extinct; two, a post-human civilization is extremely unlikely to run simulations; three, we are already living in a simulation created by a post-human society.

"We can estimate the amount of computing power available to mature civilization and we can also estimate how much it would take to simulate the human brain or, for that matter, 7 billion human brains. There would be many, many more simulated civilizations like ours, living in these ancestor simulations, than there would be original versions of us," he told listeners of the BBC's Infinite Monkey Cage in 2017.

The counterargument is that computer scientists are still a long way off from being able to simulate a human brain, and even if they did, simulating thought is not the same as thinking. Equally, simulating consciousness is not the same as being conscious.

In 1999 – co-incidentally the year The Matrix was released – philosopher of mind John Searle used a biological analogy: a computer can simulate digestion, but that doesn't mean it can eat pizza.

Writeup Paper
 
He said the fact that physical systems show a tendency towards declining disorder showed "excess information is removed" and "resembles the process of a computer deleting or compressing waste code to save storage space and optimize power consumption. And as a result supports the idea that we're living in a simulation."
:lol: This is where it gets far fetched. Now that we can build computers, everything must be based on that model. When first built watches, god was a watch maker. When the sun and thunder and lightning were important aspects of life, they were the gods.
 
We count to 4 differently to how we count above 5

For more than a century, researchers have known that people are generally very good at eyeballing quantities of four or fewer items. But performance at sizing up numbers drops markedly — becoming slower and more prone to error — in the face of larger numbers.

Now scientists have discovered why: the human brain uses one mechanism to assess four or fewer items and a different one for when there are five or more. The findings, obtained by recording the neuron activity of 17 human participants, settle a long-standing debate on how the brain estimates how many objects a person sees. The results were published in Nature Human Behaviour on 2 October.

The limits of the human ability to estimate large quantities have puzzled many generations of scientists. In an 1871 Nature article, economist and logician William Stanley Jevons described his investigations into his own counting skills and concluded “that the number five is beyond the limit of perfect discrimination, by some persons at least”.

Some researchers have argued that the brain uses a single estimation system, one that is simply less precise for higher numbers. Others hypothesize that the performance discrepancy arises from there being two separate neuronal systems to quantify objects. But experiments have failed to determine which model is correct.

Then, a team of researchers had a rare opportunity to record the activity of individual neurons inside the brains of people who were awake. All were being treated for seizures at the University Hospital Bonn in Germany, and had microelectrodes inserted in their brains in preparation for surgery.

The authors showed 17 participants images of anywhere from zero to nine dots on a screen for half a second, and asked them whether they had seen an odd or even number of items. As expected, the participants’ answers were much more precise when they saw four or fewer dots.

The researchers had already learned from previous research that there are specialized neurons associated with specific numbers of items. Some fire primarily when presented with one object, others when presented with two objects and so forth.

“The higher the preferred number, the less selective these neurons were,” says co-author Andreas Nieder, an animal physiologist at the University of Tubingen in Germany. For example, neurons specific to three would only fire in response to that number, whereas neurons that prefer eight would respond to eight but also to seven and nine. As a result, people made more mistakes when trying to quantify a larger number of objects.

This suggests two distinct ‘number systems’ in the brain. Nieder was surprised, as he previously thought that there was only one mechanism. “I had a hard time believing that there’s really this dividing line. But, based on these data, I must accept it,” he says.

So are there 4 lights, or 5?
 
New information physics theory is evidence 'we're living in a simulation,' says author

Going by the fact that Elon Musk said there was only one in a billion chance that the world was not simulated, we might save ourselves a lot of time and assume that on this occasion, as on so many others, the 52-year-old rocket bro has made a beeline for the wrong end of the stick.

Nonetheless, in the developing field of information physics, one researcher claims to have found evidence that we are all living in our own rendition of The Matrix, minus the latex, bad acting, and dated cyberpunk references.

Melvin Vopson, associate professor of physics at the UK's Portsmouth University, has developed theory in the field of information physics and applied it to predict genetic mutations in organisms, including viruses, and help judge their potential consequences.

The basis of the theory is analogous to the second law of thermodynamics, which includes the idea that the disorder within an isolated system will increase unless it is acted on by some external force or condition.

In information physics – the idea that studying the information inherent in physical systems produces fruitful results – the trend goes the other way.

Dr Vopson, who spent some of his career at disk drive giant Seagate, has developed the second law of infodynamics, which says that the entropy – or disorder – within information stays the same or decreases over time. The idea has implications across a number of fields, he claims in a paper published by the American Institute of Physics.

In biology, the theory suggests that genetic mutations follow a pattern governed by information entropy, with implications in genetic research, evolutionary biology, genetic therapies, pharmacology, virology, and more. In atomic physics, the paper claims, electrons arrange themselves in a way that minimizes their information entropy, helping science to understand the stability of chemicals. Lastly, in cosmology, the second law of infodynamics can be used to support the idea of an expanding universe.

"The paper also provides an explanation for the prevalence of symmetry in the universe," said Dr Vopson in a prepared statement.

"Symmetry principles play an important role with respect to the laws of nature, but until now there has been little explanation as to why that could be. My findings demonstrate that high symmetry corresponds to the lowest information entropy state, potentially explaining nature's inclination towards it."

But Dr Vopson takes another step, which is where it gets interesting, or far-fetched, depending on your perspective.

He said the fact that physical systems show a tendency towards declining disorder showed "excess information is removed" and "resembles the process of a computer deleting or compressing waste code to save storage space and optimize power consumption. And as a result supports the idea that we're living in a simulation."

In 2003, Nick Bostrom, Oxford University professor and director of the Future of Humanity Institute, published an argument [PDF] suggesting that the future of humanity lies in one of three scenarios: one, that humans will become extinct; two, a post-human civilization is extremely unlikely to run simulations; three, we are already living in a simulation created by a post-human society.

"We can estimate the amount of computing power available to mature civilization and we can also estimate how much it would take to simulate the human brain or, for that matter, 7 billion human brains. There would be many, many more simulated civilizations like ours, living in these ancestor simulations, than there would be original versions of us," he told listeners of the BBC's Infinite Monkey Cage in 2017.

The counterargument is that computer scientists are still a long way off from being able to simulate a human brain, and even if they did, simulating thought is not the same as thinking. Equally, simulating consciousness is not the same as being conscious.

In 1999 – co-incidentally the year The Matrix was released – philosopher of mind John Searle used a biological analogy: a computer can simulate digestion, but that doesn't mean it can eat pizza.

Writeup Paper

To me that sounds like an Intelligent Design argument with "God" substituted by "simulation".
 

Climate crisis: Coca-Cola trial to make bottle tops from CO2 emissions​

Coca-Cola has unveiled plans to make its bottle tops from carbon dioxide taken out of the atmosphere.
The firm - one of the world's biggest users of plastic - is funding a three-year trial at Swansea University as part of the company's target of net zero by 2040.
Much of its current plastic packaging is made cheaply, from fossil fuels.
But it aims to "capture" CO2 from the air, or from factory emissions, to produce a key ingredient for plastics.

"The plastic we make today releases a lot of carbon dioxide into the environment," said the project's principle investigator, Professor Enrico Andreoli.
"Our starting material is carbon dioxide," he said, "so we entirely de-fossilise the process and make plastic free from fossil fuels and fossil carbon."

Plastic free from fossil fuels​

Prof Andreoli, an industrial chemist, said the "magic happens" in a small black electrode where an electric charge passes through a mixture of CO2 and water, producing ethylene, a key ingredient in the flexible type of plastic used in bottle tops.
"We want to prove the technology in the laboratory works," he said, explaining how "success" would suggest ways to scale up the process.

Coca-Cola's goal is to use "captured" CO2 as a resource, taking it either from the air near its factories or directly from its own smoke stacks.
Ethylene is currently made as cheap by-product of refining petrochemicals, with fossil fuels heated to more than 800C (1472F), "cracking" off the molecules needed to make plastic.

Reducing carbon footprint​

The process produced more than 260 million tonnes of CO2 emissions in 2020, or nearly 1% of the world's total CO2 emissions, according to the climate tracking group the Global Carbon Project.
Craig Twyford, director of Coca-Cola's venturing division for Europe and the Pacific, said the company's promise for a 30% reduction in its carbon footprint by 2030 will mostly come from using more recycled plastics.

"From 2030 to 2040 we need to start making the more radical bets... looking at lots of different technologies.
"If the human race starts drawing down CO2 in large quantities, what useful things can we do with it?
"We could potentially use it to carbonate our drinks. Or, we could use it - as we are with Swansea - to make some of our packaging."
In a similar project, the company is funding research in California to turn CO2 into an artificial sugar.
https://www.bbc.com/news/uk-wales-67060151
 
Deep asleep? You can still follow simple commands, study finds

Scientists once considered sleep to be like a shade getting drawn over a window between the brain and the outside world: when the shade is closed, the brain stops reacting to outside stimuli.

A study published on 12 October in Nature Neuroscience suggests that there might be periods during sleep when that shade is partially open. Depending on what researchers said to them, participants in the study would either smile or frown on cue in certain phases of sleep.

A few years ago, however, Oudiette began questioning this definition after she and her team conducted an experiment in which they were able to communicate with people who are aware that they are dreaming while they sleep — otherwise known as lucid dreamers. During these people’s dreams, experimenters were able to ask questions and get responses through eye and facial-muscle movements.

Karen Konkoly, who was a coauthor on that study and a cognitive scientist at Northwestern University in Evanston, Illinois, says that after that paper came out, “it was a big open question in our minds whether communication would be possible with non-lucid dreamers”.

So Oudiette continued with the work. In her latest study, she and her colleagues observed 27 people with narcolepsy — characterized by daytime sleepiness and a high frequency of lucid dreams — and 22 people without the condition. While they were sleeping, participants were repeatedly asked to frown or smile. All of them responded accurately to at least 70% of these prompts.

Overall response rates were higher for all participants during REM (rapid eye movement) sleep, when the deepest sleep occurs but the brain remains quite active, than during other sleep stages. The researchers tracked participants’ brain activity during the experiments using electroencephalography (EEG), which captures signals from electrodes placed along a person’s scalp.

Writeup Paper


Spoiler Legend :
Participants with narcolepsy went through five 20-min naps during the same day. In each nap, periods with stimulation (ON) alternated, every minute, with periods when no stimulus was presented (OFF). During the ON periods, participants were presented with words and pseudo-words and asked to either frown (corrugator muscle contractions) or smile three times (zygomatic muscle contractions) in response to the stimuli. Stimuli were presented every 10 s (±1 s). Following each nap, participants were asked to report whether (1) they had any dream, (2) they were lucid and (3) they recalled any words presented during the nap. Immediately after this debriefing, participants performed a forced-choice ‘old/new’ recognition task. Healthy participants went through the exact same procedure except that they had a single 100-min nap.
 
I've never been able to achieve lucid dreaming. Realizing I'm dreaming always wakes me up.
 
I've never been able to achieve lucid dreaming. Realizing I'm dreaming always wakes me up.
I seem to only have lucid dreams when I'm sleeping on my back.
 
I have slight facial blindness, so the few times I achieve lucid dreaming, I summon some people for my personal movie adventure and they are automatically in the uncanny valley.

So I try to fix their faces and just about when I get it right I wake up from concentrating too hard. :sad:
 
I do not understand this at all, but it sounds cool.

‘Dark photons’ hint at nature of dark matter

Indirect evidence of ‘dark photons’ has been spotted in the shrapnel of thousands of particle-smashing experiments. This could provide a glimpse into the mysterious nature of dark matter — which seems to make up most of the mass of the universe but is almost impossible to observe, other than by its gravitational effects.

“The story is something like this: there could be an additional dark sector, where dark matter resides, and that couples weakly to the ordinary sector – in this case, via the mixing of a gauge boson, the dark photon, with the ordinary neutral gauge bosons,” says Wagner, referring to the photons, W and Z bosons that carry the electromagnetic and weak forces. “Such a gauge boson may couple in a relevant way to the dark matter and, in general, to a hypothetical dark sector.”

In the latest study, the Adelaide-led team, which also included researchers at the Jefferson Lab in Virginia, US, performed a global quantum chromodynamics (QCD) analysis of high-energy scattering data within the Jefferson Lab Angular Momentum (JAM) framework. The researchers demonstrated that when they try to explain the results of deep inelastic scattering (DIS) experiments, a model that incorporates a dark photon is preferred over the competing Standard Model hypothesis at a significance of 6.5σ.

“[DIS] is the process where a probe like an electron, muon or neutrino scatters from a proton with such high transfer of energy and momentum (hence deep) that it smashes the proton into pieces (hence inelastic),” Thomas explains. “If you sum over all the pieces, you can determine the distribution of momentum of the quarks within the original proton.”


Dark halo: A Hubble Space Telescope image of the inner region of the galaxy cluster Abell 1689. Though dark matter cannot be imaged directly, it can be mapped by plotting the plethora of arcs produced by the light from background galaxies that is warped by the foreground cluster’s gravitational field, indicated by the blue overlay.
 
Top Bottom