Ordnael
Emperor
I always try to find frogs to play with when I am holidaying at a river! Got some neat pictures and videos with a small frog sitting at my leg trying to catch passing flies!
Last edited:
These findings overturn long-held assumptions of European genetic dominance and show that native South Americans are of Asian descent.
I think much of their point was not that they were of Asian descent, but that over the course of their travels and time, their genetics changed and became less robust.As, well, this isn't news. I'm pretty sure we've had clear genetic evidence that indigenous Americans are descended from Asian populations for quite some time now. And, frankly, even without genetics, it's kinda obvious, given that they got to American from Asia and have material culture and potentially linguistic connections to other Asian groups.
The remains of the woman were found in 1988 in the Margaux cave near Dinant in Wallonia. Scientists from Ghent University and specialist artists from the Kennis & Kennis studio reconstructed her face based on anatomical, genetic and archaeological research.
(...)
Her living environment was also accurately reconstructed based on archaeological data and scientific models. Campsites, hunting techniques and means of transport bring the world of more than 10,000 years ago back to life.
The reconstruction is part of the travelling exhibition Face to Face with Prehistory, which will tour Belgium and the Netherlands from August. In the meantime, the public are invited to choose a name for the woman.
Demonstrating the degree of precision achieved, the two spacecraft use their formation flying time to create artificial total solar eclipses in orbit – they align with the Sun so that the 1.4 m large disc carried by the Occulter spacecraft covers the bright disc of the Sun for the Coronagraph spacecraft, casting a shadow of 8 cm across onto its optical instrument, ASPIICS.
This instrument, short for Association of Spacecraft for Polarimetric and Imaging Investigation of the Corona of the Sun, was developed for ESA by an industrial consortium led by Centre Spatial de Liège, Belgium. When its 5 cm aperture is covered by the shadow, the instrument captures images of the solar corona uninterrupted by the Sun’s bright light.
Observing the corona is crucial for revealing solar wind, the continuous flow of matter from the Sun into outer space. It is also necessary for understanding the workings of coronal mass ejections (CMEs), explosions of particles sent out by the Sun almost every day, especially during high activity periods.
Look at this autocensor nonsense @ThunderfallFairy wrens teach secret passwords to their unborn chicks to tell them apart from cu.ckoo impostors
Not Exactly Rocket Science
By Ed Yong
Nov 8, 2012 4:00 PMNov 19, 2019 5:05 PM
[The banned word is Cu ckoo]
![]()
In Australia, a pair of superb fairy-wrens return to their nest with food for their newborn chick. As they arrive, the chick makes its begging call. It’s hard to see in the darkness of the domed nest, but the parents know that something isn’t right. Whatever’s in their nest, it’s not their chick. It doesn’t’ know the secret password. They abandon it, flying off to start a new nest and a new family somewhere else. It was a good call. The bird in their nest was a Horsfield’s bronze-****oo. These birds are “brood parasites” – they lay their eggs in those of other birds, passing on their parenting duties to some unwitting surrogates. The bronze-****oo egg looks very much like a fairy-wren egg, although it tends to hatch earlier. The ****oo chick then ejects its foster siblings from the nest, so it can monopolise its foster parents’ attention. But fairy-wrens have a way of telling their chicks apart from ****oos. Diane Colombelli-Negrel from Flinders University in Australia has shown that mothers sing a special tune to their eggs before they’ve hatched. This “incubation call” contains a special note that acts like a familial password. The embryonic chicks learn it, and when they hatch, they incorporate it into their begging calls. Horsfield’s bronze-****oos lay their eggs too late in the breeding cycle for their chicks to pick up the same notes. They can’t learn the password in time, and their identities can be rumbled.
This is one of many incredible adaptations in the long-running battle between birds and their brood parasite. As these evolutionary arms races continue, the parasites typically become ever better mimics, and the hosts typically become ever more discerning parents. This battle usually plays out before the eggs hatch. If the parents can recognise the parasitic eggs, they’ll eject or destroy them. If they can’t, they often end up feeding the parasite regardless of what it looks like. This is why the common ****oo has an egg that closely matches that of a reed warbler, but the ****oo chick is a huge, grey monster that looks completely unlike a warbler chick. But the fairy-wrens are different. In 2003, Naomi Langmore found that they will abandon 40 percent of nests that only have a Horsfield bronze-****oo chick in it, suggesting that they can indeed recognise these interlopers. Now, Colombelli-Negrel has discovered how they do it. She kept 15 nests under constant audio surveillance, and discovered that fairy-wrens call to their unhatched chicks, using a two-second trill with 19 separate elements to it. They call once every four minutes while sitting on their eggs, starting on the 9^th day of incubation and carrying on for a week until the eggs hatch. When Colombelli-Negrel recorded the chicks after they hatched, she heard that their begging call included a single unique note lifted from mum’s incubation call. This note varies a lot between different fairy-wren broods. It’s their version of a surname, a signature of identity that unites a family. The females even teach these calls to their partners, by using them in their own begging calls when the males return to the nest with food.
These signature calls aren’t innate. The chicks’ calls more precisely matched those of their mother if she sang more frequently while she was incubating. And when Colombelli-Negrel swapped some eggs between different clutches, she found that the chicks made signature calls that matches those of their foster parents rather than those of their biological ones. It’s something they learn while still in their eggs. This also explains why bronze-****oos don’t make the same calls. The female bronze-****oo tends to deposit her eggs while a fairy-wren’s clutch is around 12 days old. At this point, they’re just a couple of days away from hatching. “The ****oo embryo appears to have insufficient time to correctly learn the password note,” says Sonia Kleindorfer, who led the study. When they hatch, the fairy-wrens can tell that something isn’t right. They spend less time feeding their alien chick, more time making alarm calls, and more time scanning the surrounding area, possibly on the lookout for more ****oos. In many cases, they abandon the intruder to make a fresh start elsewhere.
This isn’t a clear win for the fairy-wrens. Colombelli-Negrel found very low levels of ****oo parasitism in her study, but Langmore previously showed that only 40 percent of the wrens abandon their ****oo-infested nests. “There appear to be cyclical fluctuations in ****oo prevalance in host nests across years,” says Kleindorfer. This might be because the local wrens become better or worse at recognising their signature notes, or the ****oos become better or worse at mimicking them. “Our study provides a testable framework to explain some of this variation.”
Reference: Colombelli-Negrel, Hauber, Robertson, Sulloway, Hoi, Griggio & Kleindorfer. 2012. Embryonic Learning of Vocal Passwords in Superb Fairy-Wrens Reveals Intruder ****oo Nestlings. Current Biology (2012), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.025More on brood parasites: